Edward Jones and Keith Davis proposed in 1965 that people conclude others when their activities are deliberate rather than unintentional. People look for a correlation between a person's motivations and behaviors when they observe others behave in a certain way. Individuals subsequently conclude based on the degree of choice, the expectedness of the behavior, and the outcomes of that behavior.
A judgment that a person's personality resembles or corresponds to his or her behavior is known as a correspondent inference, which is also known as a correspondent trait inference. For instance, we have made or drawn a correspondent inference if we observe Taliyah acting pleasantly and conclude that she has a friendly nature. Alternatively, if we observe Carl acting aggressively and deduce that he is aggressive, we have made a correspondent inference. Sometimes it makes sense to assume that people's behaviors are consistent with their personalities while others do not. The idea of correspondent inference describes when it is fair to assume that a person's behavior and personality match up.
The theory of corresponding inference states that the objective of the attribution process is to conclude another person based on observed behavior and the purpose that resulted in them, i.e., to determine that the observed behavior and the intention correspond to some underlying stable personality attribute or quality within the person, i.e., a disposition. In another way, "correspondent inference" refers to the perceiver's conclusion that an actor's behavior is due to or relates to a specific attribute largely stable across time. Such an inference can be demonstrated simply by attributing someone's aggressive behavior to their feature of hostility. Thus, behavior directly reveals underlying dispositions. Dispositional attributions, however, frequently assign a group of "broad" features to the person, notwithstanding the dearth of supporting empirical data. Understanding and predicting another person's behavior is allegedly made possible by being aware of their dispositional characteristics.
As a perceiver, determining which of the various impacts of a person's behavior was intended depends on several variables, such as how common the effects were, how desirable they were on a social level, and how closely the behavior fit the normative perspective.
The principle of non-common effects states that when a behavior has an exceptional or non-common impact that any other behavior cannot create, we can infer that the behavior relates to an underlying disposition.
It is thought that when behavior is freely chosen, it is because of internal (dispositional) causes.
When a person's actions have socially negative consequences, we often assume that their behavior indicates their underlying temperament. Engaging in socially desirable behaviors does not reflect any particular personality; it merely indicates our desire to appear normal and similar to others. Low socially desirable behaviors, however, are implied as a result of a personal trait.
The perceiver assesses the behavior's normativity to conclude that the behavior is a product of the person's disposition. The behavior often anticipated by a person in a specific social context is normativeness. When a behavior deviates from the situation's social norms, it appears that the individual in question freely chose the behavior and was not subjected to any pressure.
Accidental behavior is likely to be attributed to circumstances or outside factors, whereas purposeful behavior is likely to be attributed to the person's nature.
Furthermore, Jones and Davis (1965) contended that socially acceptable behavior typically does not disclose personal preferences. Alternately, actions that defy societal conventions are attributed to personal attributes. According to correspondent inference theory, we are most likely to conclude that other people's behavior reflects their stable qualities and dispositional elements when that behavior is freely chosen, has distinctive, non-common outcomes, and has low social desirability.
The propensity to draw correspondent inferences may vary between situations to such an extent that it cannot be characterized as an individual difference due to the various factors contributing to correspondence bias. Additionally, the propensity to draw corresponding inferences probably varies greatly depending on observers' inferences. For instance, observers' abilities may impact judgments of ability, whereas observers' levels of disgust sensitivity may have a stronger impact on moral judgments. The existence of a shared inferential corrective mechanism underlies correspondence bias, which is one element pointing to the tendency to draw correspondent inferences as a persistent individual difference. Gilbert et al. (1988) hypothesized that people's initial dispositional predictions about another person's behavior are the outcome of a mostly automatic process that requires little effort or conscious attention and seems to hold across cultural boundaries. These instinctive inferences are corrected to account for situational limits due to a more regulated and purposeful procedure that necessitates the subject's presence and active use of cognitive resources. As a result, there may be a significant individual difference in the tendency to draw corresponding inferences depending on one's capacity and desire to invest cognitive resources in conducting that correction process.
Some evidence supports the corresponding inference theory. However, social psychologists have focused on how frequently people break from the idea. People often assume that personality and behavior are somewhat correlated, even though they know that when a scenario tends to cause people to behave in a certain manner, the behavior is not revealing about personality. Therefore, even if they know that winning a car usually makes people happy, if they observe a contestant acting joyful after winning a car, they may conclude that the contestant has a generally happy nature. The propensity to infer that personality corresponds to behavior even when the situation appears to explain the behavior is known as correspondence bias. The propensity to assume that actors and actresses have personalities consistent with their portrayals is an excellent illustration of correspondence bias. We might assume that Arnold Schwarzenegger is somewhat aggressive even though we know he portrays an aggressive character.