Reports that variables thought to influence lexical sorting, such as frequency, repetition, connection, and orthographic legality, and variables thought to influence reply sorting, such as predictability. Even variables thought to impact discourse-level sorting, like congruence with a global context, that everything similarly modifies a negative-going ERP element (N400) evident between 200 as well as 500 ms after encoding cast doubt on the lexical/post-lexical distinction.
It is easy to underestimate the brain power required to process language because adults do it so often, rapidly, and effortlessly. One example is the complex sensory signal that must be processed for a listener to ford a spoken sentence. A person's ability to understand speech depends on their ability to process information across multiple time and frequency scales. Differentiating the consonants that make words up, like "pat" and "bat," requires an awareness of timing differences on the order of milliseconds. In addition, the listener needs to be able to recognize tonal patterns developing placed above white seconds or even mins to understand, for instance, when she is being requested information or ordered to do something
Moreover, this same sorting of the sensor's signal is just the beginning, even as the listener must then look for familiar patterns inside the input, such as phrases and grammatical structure, retrieve various types of information associated with each, as well as combine people appropriately. Despite popular belief, lexical sorting does not occur before post-lexical ERP effects. Further evidence against the assumption of automaticity in lexical sorting comes from studies showing how their context may dampen the impact of words.
These results point to an associative structure for storing and retrieving information about words, with the frequency and time since the last usage playing a role in this structure. Furthermore, lexical judgment task research has shown two effects that provide strong hints at crucial limits on the design of the system used to identify words.
This shows that words lead to faster reactions than no words do. The second is known as the "nonword legality effect," which shows that responses to pseudowords with a valid orthographic structure are slower than nonwords made up of random letter sequences.
Given that one must infer from measurements taken at the end state of sorting (the vocal output or even the button push) rather than more immediate access to intermediate phases of sorting, the precise nature of the time course for lexical access is already a challenging problem.
The N400 could provide us with a glimpse into the process of lexical access at a time when pseudowords are being separated from genuine words and when factors like regularity, repetition, and meaning relatedness have a large influence. Nonetheless, the N400 is susceptible to aspects most consider post-lexical and those that impact lexical access. One of the greatest predictors of N400 loudness for the word in a phrase is the word's cloze likelihood.
Because these promise to provide more immediate access to these intermediary phases, current neuroimaging methods are fascinating. Operational magnetic resonance imaging (functional MRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) can show which brain regions are engaged during certain experimental tasks by monitoring neuronal activity's oxygenation or blood flow. Then, differences and similarities in activation patterns could be utilized to make sense of inconsistencies between observable behavioral tasks that are both assumed to index lexical availability.
The N400 amplitudes are greatest for out-of-the-ordinary elements like semantic anomalies and are at their lowest for words that fit their context well and have high cloze probability. The magnitude of the N400 signal often decreases as the target word is more likely to be predicted by the context immediately before it.
Conventional wisdom is that lexical sorting takes place quickly and automatically at the outset of language learning. It is generally agreed that post-lexical sorting is slower, more restricted, and dependent on the results of the lexical sorting. As a result, it is anticipated that lexical variables will impact word sorting before post-lexical factors, will do so by a qualitatively distinct mechanism from post-lexical variables, and will be more resilient than post-lexical variables.
When we lack more global context, lexical factors like frequency and lexical connection become more important. The N400 amplitude is more sensitive to frequency at the beginning of a phrase than at the closing. The amplitude of the N400 is influenced when the reader is aided in constructing the message-level representation by lexical association & contextual congruence. However, contextual congruity wins out when the benefits of contextual congruity and lexical association for building the message-level representation are at odds with one another. Overall, higher-order contextual factors—semantic or syntactic—moderate lexical effects' presumed automaticity and independence.
A scene is more than a collection of items, much like a term. Understanding what is said requires retrieving lexical information and integrating it with higher-order linguistic representations, local context cues, and previous knowledge activated by discursive interpretations (Coulson, in press). To properly understand "water" in the phrase "Everyone had so much fun plunging from the branch into the pond, we added some," it is not enough to know that it is a mass noun and a colorless, odorless fluid essential for plant and animal life. To fully appreciate the joke, one needs grammatical knowledge (gapping) to infer that the water is flowing into the pool, contextual knowledge to consider that agents are diving from a shrub into a pool that may or may not contain water, as well as background knowledge to realize that pools without water are less fun to dive into. The brain uses orthography, recency, regularity, lexical association, and congruity just at the sentence and discourse tiers simultaneously, which also seems well-suited for message construction, considering the complexity of situational integration and the need to combine info from multiple levels of analysis quickly. A recent study shows that the brain analyses words in context, not words plus context.