Clinical neuropsychology requires the integration of knowledge bases from psychology, psychometrics, neuroscience, clinical neuropsychology, and psychiatry. This class will provide an overview of how neuropsychologists measure brain function. First, we will examine the topic from a developmental standpoint, demonstrating how neuropsychologists assess small preschool children, older children, young adults, and seniors.
The brain is an ever-changing organ that acts considerably differently at various periods of life. During these stages, assessment issues change, as do the frequency and characteristics of many brain illnesses. A 3-year-old child's behavioural exam cannot employ the same materials and techniques as a 40-year-old adult's evaluation.
The clinical practice of employing tests and other behavioural evaluation tools to establish the status of brain function is known as neuropsychological assessment. It is founded on the concept that the brain is the organ of behaviour; hence, the brain's state may be assessed using behavioural metrics. Over many years of research, several techniques have been discovered to have a specific sensitivity to changes in brain activity, and these processes have been known as psychological tests.
A neuropsychological test is thus characterized as a behavioural approach that is highly sensitive to the brain state. While every deliberate behaviour affects the brain, neuropsychological tests give the clearest displays of behaviour that show whether or not the brain is operating correctly. Various factors can cause brain dysfunction, including genetic predisposition, developmental abnormalities, physical injury, toxic or infectious agent exposure, systemic diseases (e.g., vascular disorders, cancer, metabolic disorders), and progressive disorders that affect the central nervous system tissue, such as multiple sclerosis.
One can divide neuropsychological assessment into two areas: comprehensive and specialized assessment. Standard test batteries, such as the Halstead Reitan or Luria Nebraska batteries, are commonly used in comprehensive assessments. A comprehensive assessment usually assesses all areas assessed by specialized assessments, although it may just undertake specialized assessments to answer the referral question. This is a contentious issue in the profession. However, the more effective strategy is likely to be heavily tied to the context in which one works and the characte ristics of its patient group.
There are three functional domains of particular interest in this age range −
Attention
Memory
Executive Function
The purpose of a neuropsychologist is to push clinicians to build notions regarding brain-behaviour interactions in this group and offer a foundation for clinicians to establish hypotheses in their clinical assessments and effect appropriate therapies. The goal is to present a partial set of tests and measurements rather than analyze the functional areas for which a prudent test selection might be made. Whether one should refer to the "neuropsychology" of infants and young children is controversial. The questions that need to be answered include the following −
How does one reliably assess and evaluate brain-behaviour relationships in the new-born, neonate, or very young child?
If this is possible, how practical would such an evaluation be?
Is the methodology used by paediatric neuropsychologists applicable to the youngest ages? and
If so, with what degree of reliability or validity?
Which variables are traceable to the very youngest ages?
Which will result in long-lasting (i.e., adult) cognitive compromise?
What interventions can be applied in these very early years to lessen the impact of early insults?
Many practitioners are understandably hesitant to support language that may be inaccurate or unsuitable since these and other related concerns still need to be solved. What is not debatable is that paediatric and psychological specialists frequently encounter infants and young children with developmental delays or cognitive deficiencies caused by an underlying neurodevelopmental abnormality or a documented neurological disease or disorder that occurred during the earliest stages of growth and development. Several convergent advances have resulted in a greater realization that these etiological variables exist and have a substantial effect on the child's subsequent cognitive outcome. These include
A better definition of the unique developmental concepts applicable to infants and young children.
Finely detailed analyses of normal and abnormal brain development from experimental studies of nonhumans.
Major advances in neurodiagnostic techniques.
An expanding clinical and research literature on human developmental studies.
An increase in societal attention to the needs of infants and young children, with an emphasis on infants and young children.
Infants' and very young children's cognitive and social-emotional development is distinct. Compared to older children, this age range is linked with less differentiation of some functional regions, early developmental constructs that are less dominating, and more variability of performance.
Understanding the essential antecedent behaviours during these early years helps the neuropsychologist make early predictions about eventual patterns of strength and weakness, such as in attention, memory, and executive function. Symbol representation and imitation in learning are two essential principles for understanding brain development in a very young kid.
Knowledge of these principles improves our understanding of developmental progress and provides a firmer foundation for understanding cognitive and social development. The social environment also has an important impact. The symbolic representation uses symbols to represent both the exterior and internal worlds, and it has addressed crucial neuropsychological areas such as executive self-regulation and memory.
Imitation in the learning process is also an important developmental concept for new-borns and young children. Although social learning theory has long held that behaviour is an important component of learning, imitation behaviours, and so on, they are substantially more obvious and overt in new-borns and early children. From a neuropsychological standpoint, imitation can be an adaptive kind of stimuli-constrained behaviour, which can be problematic at older ages.
Clinical practice approaches developed last year have mostly targeted older children and adolescents, and there has yet to be a comparable emphasis on new-borns and early kids. Theoretical models established for older children and adults have yet to have much influence on clinical practice with young children. The lack of a model of cognitive development that spans the full age range is restricting and contributes to the relative lack of attention paid to the youngest children. A comprehensive model would −
Provide continuity in understanding developmental progress, or lack thereof, in disorders affecting neurological development.
Encourage a broader consideration of function and a more diverse selection of methodologies, that is, not limiting assessment to general cognitive development alone.
Encourage a broader consideration of function and a more diverse selection of methodologies.
Allow for earlier definition and separation of functions during an active period of developmental gain or delay.
Promote early and specific suggestions that can directly affect the developmental path and lessen the impact of an impediment to development.
Offer a foundation for assessing the efficacy of treatment suggestions.
Encourage the expansion of current information in the early years concerning the natural history of normal and pathological brain-behavior linkages.
Encourage the development of new ideas and procedures that can lead to improved science and practice. For example, to more accurately examine etiological variables or to improve knowledge of later consequences of early damage or sickness. The strength that such a model would provide explains why the emphasis should be shifting to very young children and a thorough grasp of the whole life cycle.
All trustworthy and accurate data accessible to paediatric neuropsychologists, including the history, direct and indirect observations of behaviour, and performance on specified tests, must be employed to examine new-borns and young children. Although the evaluation of a person at any age should only be based partially on test results, working with new-borns and early children necessitates the utilization of numerous sources of data to an even larger extent.
Infants' and very young children's cognitive and social-emotional development is distinct. Compared to older children, this age range is linked with less differentiation of some functional regions, early developmental constructs that are less dominating, and more variability of performance. While the approach to the neuropsychological examination of infants and young children is similar in general to that used in the evolution of older individuals, there are differences in terms of the existence of a body of knowledge regarding the cognitive and social-emotional development process in this age group and brain-behaviour relationships in normal and abnormal developmental conditions. There is less reliance on standardized test measures to assess cognitive and social-emotional development.